wasser-sparen-in-der-wasserkrise-hilft-gegen-klimawandel_artikel-teaser_800x450px_c
Insights

Saving water in a water crisis: confronting climate change with science

Behavioral Finance
Emerging Markets
Sustainable Value
Insights

Pubblicato il 10.05.2021 CEST

Cape Town’s water crisis shows how we can solve environmental problems by encouraging virtuous habits. A reportage on saving water, holding crucial lessons on how we can deal with climate change, scientist say.

By
Alex Lee,

British science journalist and permanent freelancer for Wired UK. We publish his report here as part of our Publishing Partnership with Wired UK.

 

  

How it all began

In 2017, Cape Town was on the verge of running out of water. Facing the city’s worst drought on record, it was able to back away from the cliff edge thanks, in part, to a number of smart behavioural interventions.

Despite the municipal government implementing a number of measures to try and get its 4.7 million residents to cut down their water consumption, more than 60 per cent of Capetonians were still ignoring the call to action. The city then introduced restrictions on the amount of water each resident was allowed to use per day, dialed down water pressure, hiked up tariffs and fines, and installed water restriction devices at households who weren’t complying.

Despite all that, consumption rates were still high.

Habits change only slowly

That finally changed in 2018, when the city was faced with the real possibility that its homes’ taps could run dry. In January, the government launched a city-wide water saving campaign underpinned by behavioural science. This was the beginning of this success story.

Ever since, academics have been poring over data collected during the water crisis to find out how exactly it succeeded. Confronting the Cape Town water crisis was, in a way, a lot like confronting climate change:

Better understanding how this water crisis was solved might hold some important clues about how to handle a much bigger crisis: the climate change.

 

“Because the drought approached and crept up on us, many people weren’t really sufficiently worried”, says Thinus Booysen, an engineering professor at Stellenbosch University in South Africa.

This is something that psychologists call “present bias” and leads to people prioritising present-day needs over future ones. Booysen observed in a retrospective paper published in 2019 that even though the government continued to limit people’s water usage to 100 litres, and then to 82 litres, residents were still using more water than advised, possibly due to confusing messaging from the government. Even though the municipality applied considerable water fines and tariffs, they didn’t have a huge impact on the consumption habits of Cape Town’s wealthier individuals. “People who could afford to just kept on using water”, Booysen says.

“Day Zero” was like a wake-up call

Then, in January 2018, the municipal government asked residents to cut down its water usage to 50 litres a day. It announced that if Capetonians didn’t act to avert the water crisis, the taps would be turned off on April 21 – an ominous date known as “Day Zero”.

It was a wakeup call for all of Cape Town’s residents. That same month, the government collaborated with the University of Cape Town’s Environmental Policy Research Unit (EPRU) to roll out a series of measures to subtly nudge people into changing their behaviour.

  • More about the EPRU at the University of Cape Town

    The EPRU had already been investigating how it could nudge people to conserve more water prior to the drought. Between 2015 and 2016, it had conducted an extensive study on 400,000 households in Cape Town aimed at finding out what the best way was to nudge people into reducing their water consumption. During the study, researchers implemented pro-social measures like comparing a household’s water usage to its neighbours’, and financial nudges informing people of how much money they would save – or lose – depending on their water usage.

 

 

Social recognition as a key incentive

One of the most significant nudges was the publication of the names and addresses of the top water savers on the city’s website. Those targeted with this message – a mix of demographics - reduced their water use by 1.9 per cent.

“We found that the high income groups responded better to the pro-environmental, prosocial, public good – specifically the social recognition type measures – and not so much to the financial ones.”


This is the conclusion reached by Martine Visser. She’s a behavioural economist at the University of Cape Town, who spearheaded the development of the nudges behind the Day Zero campaign. “For middle and low-income households, we did see that price and financial nudges mattered more”.

At the start of the Day Zero campaign, the government rolled out an online water map devised by Visser and her team. The map publicly acknowledged households that were achieving water-saving targets. If a household reached its target, it was rewarded with a green symbol displayed on their property. The map was designed with the knowledge gained from the 2016 study, which highlighted how effective it is to recognise people socially when it came to incentivising water scarcity.

Fear motivates

Messages were also employed to help residents frame water targets as more understandable units of measurements. The city released infographics of how much water one flush of the toilet would use, or how many litres of water a two-minute shower would use. Visser says:

Making information clearer is a lesson that can be applied to the global fight against climate change more broadly.


Messaging needs to be delivered in a way that everyone can understand. “It took us almost going to the brink for the government to really understand the extent to which communication needs to improve to make people respond to the crisis”, she says.

Booysen also led the installation of smart water meters across 345 schools in the Western Cape, turning water conservation into a competition between the schools. Some schools were also given weekly report cards with their water usage, defined by multiples of bottles of coke or swimming pools. Overall, the interventions led to a 15 to 26 per cent water usage reduction.

Booysen admits that the strongest intervention was the “fear factor”. In January 2018, the municipal government announced that if Capetonians didn’t act to avert the water crisis, the taps would be turned off on April 21. Everyone would be forced to queue at one of 200 guarded collection points to receive a daily 25-litre water ration.

“That is the moment when the penny dropped,” he says. “What our analysis of smart metre water data showed is that was the exact point when usage substantially dropped”.

While Day Zero was eventually postponed indefinitely thanks to the city pulling together to conserve water, disastrous droughts as seen in Cape Town are going to become more common in a warming world. According to a study from Stanford University, man-made climate change made the Day Zero drought five to six times more likely.

Incentives plus regulations work best

The success of nudging people to make small but significant changes has been seen in a number of local communities around the world. In the Indian city of Mumbai, restaurants began serving half glasses of water after they found that patrons were only taking a few sips and throwing the rest away. In California, restaurants are not allowed to serve water unless the diners explicitly ask for it.

But Cape Town’s success story also shows that change has to happen on many levels. While citizens were trying to reduce their water consumption, the municipality was also busy building new water sources, reducing water pressure across the city, and blasting out information on the importance of water conservation over the television and radio. “Nudges can be used in a very successful, complementary way”, Visser says. But they work best when paired with regulation.

 

 

  

Advanced topic “Behavioral Psychology”

In the field of energy, environment and sustainability

 

With Toby Park, principal advisor at the British consultancy “Behavioural Insights Team”


The above are all examples of what behavioural psychologists call a “default”, a mechanism that works by taking the choice out of people’s hands - a real timesaver given we make 35,000 decisions each day.

Toby Park, principal advisor of energy, environment and sustainability at the British consultancy Behavioural Insights Team says that nudges work best when they reduce friction and make things as easy as possible for people. “Often we don’t make an active choice; we just stick with the default option. That can have really profound impacts on the energy tariff we use, whether we offset our flights, the food we eat and so on”, he explains.

Park points to another study in which food wastage was reduced in school canteens when plastic trays used to carry food were removed, forcing people to carry their plates. “It really comes down to creating a world in which the sustainable choices are the easy choices, the popular, readily available choices, the default choices”, Park says.

Pro-social nudges, such as water usage maps and utility bill comparisons have proven effective in other contexts. When energy customers living in low-value homes in the US were sent monthly letters outlining how much energy their neighbours were using, those who were overusing cut their consumption by up to two per cent.

  

  

Also relevant in the context of “behavioral economics”

  

  

  

Share

Share